Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current issue
    • Past issues
    • Collections
  • Authors & Reviewers
    • Overview for Authors
    • Preparing manuscripts
    • Submission Checklist
    • Publication Fees
    • Forms
    • Editorial Policies
    • Editorial Process
    • Patient-Oriented Research
    • Manuscript Progress
    • Submitting a letter
    • Information for Reviewers
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial board
    • Contact
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CJS
    • JAMC
    • JPN

User menu

Search

  • Advanced search
CMAJ Open
  • CMAJ JOURNALS
    • CMAJ
    • CJS
    • JAMC
    • JPN
CMAJ Open

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Content
    • Current issue
    • Past issues
    • Collections
  • Authors & Reviewers
    • Overview for Authors
    • Preparing manuscripts
    • Submission Checklist
    • Publication Fees
    • Forms
    • Editorial Policies
    • Editorial Process
    • Patient-Oriented Research
    • Manuscript Progress
    • Submitting a letter
    • Information for Reviewers
    • Open access
  • Alerts
    • Email alerts
    • RSS
  • About
    • General information
    • Staff
    • Editorial board
    • Contact
  • Subscribe to our alerts
  • RSS feeds
  • Follow CMAJ Open on Twitter
Research

Screening for developmental delay among children aged 1-4 years: a systematic review

Rachel Warren, Meghan Kenny, Teresa Bennett, Donna Fitzpatrick-Lewis, Muhammad Usman Ali, Diana Sherifali and Parminder Raina
January 25, 2016 4 (1) E20-E27; DOI: https://doi.org/10.9778/cmajo.20140121
Rachel Warren
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Meghan Kenny
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Teresa Bennett
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Donna Fitzpatrick-Lewis
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Muhammad Usman Ali
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Diana Sherifali
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Parminder Raina
Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre, and Offord Centre for Child Studies, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ont.
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Tables
  • Related Content
  • Responses
  • Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

Background: Existing guidelines on screening children less than 5 years of age for developmental delay vary. In this systematic review, we synthesized the literature on the effectiveness and harms of screening for developmental delay in asymptomatic children aged 1-4 years.

Methods: We searched MEDLINE, Embase and PsychINFO for relevant articles published to June 16, 2015. We identified studies that included children aged 1-4 years who were not at high risk of developmental delay, screened in a primary care setting. Randomized trials and controlled cohort studies were considered for benefits (cognitive, academic and functional outcomes); no restrictions on study design were imposed for the review of harms.

Results: Two studies were included. One used the Ages and Stages Questionnaire II for screening and reported significantly more referrals to early intervention in the intervention groups than in the control group (relative risk [RR] 1.95, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.49-2.54, in the intervention group with office support and RR 1.71, 95% CI 1.30-2.25, in the intervention group without office support). The time to referral was 70% shorter in the intervention group with office support (rate ratio 0.30, 95% CI 0.19-0.48) and 64% shorter in the intervention group without office support (rate ratio 0.36, 95% CI 0.23-0.59), compared with the control group. The other study used the VroegTijdige Onderkenning Ontwikkelingsstoornissen Language Screening instrument to screen children aged 15 months at enrolment for language delay. It reported no differences between groups in academic performance outcomes at age 8 years.

Interpretation: The evidence on screening for developmental delay in asymptomatic children aged 1-4 years is inconclusive. Further research with longer-term outcomes is needed to inform decisions about screening and screening intervals.

The preschool period of child development (age < 5 yr) is widely recognized as a uniquely sensitive period for the foundation of cognitive ability and related functioning. Intensive change typically occurs during this time across the domains of language, social and motor development. Intellectual disability and other developmental disabilities that often co-occur (e.g., autism spectrum disorders) frequently entail lifelong challenges with respect to daily functioning and well-being for individuals and caregivers, and are considered to be detectable during the preschool period. Many caregivers, researchers and policy-makers therefore argue that detection and intervention before the age of 5 years is essential to optimize outcomes for children and families.1-3

Screening children at risk of intellectual disability and related impairment is an important challenge for health care providers and policy-makers. Global developmental delay has been defined in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-V) as "the failure of an individual under age 5 to meet expected developmental milestones across multiple areas of intellectual functioning."4 Developmental delay may be understood as the failure to meet expected milestones across a given domain of development (e.g., cognitive, language, social or motor development). The DSM-V emphasizes the difficulty in reliably assessing intellectual and related functioning among very young children during a period of intensive and variable developmental change.4

Within pediatric health care, policy decisions that address early detection and intervention for developmental delay involve a particularly complex balance of considerations. Parents of children with developmental disorders often report a sense of urgency to intervene as quickly as possible during essential developmental windows. There is also an important tension between intervening as early as possible and not over-identifying many children as developmentally delayed (i.e., too many false-positive findings). False-positive screen results for developmental delay can lead to unnecessary stigma, caregiver anxiety and excess cost, whereas false-negative results can lead to delays in treatment.

Existing guidelines and recommendations for screening children for developmental delay vary. In 1994, the Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination (now the Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care) found fair evidence to assess developmental milestones at each well-baby visit.5 In another guideline in the same year,6 the task force found good evidence to recommend against the use of the Denver Developmental Screening Test7 in asymptomatic preschool children, as well as insufficient evidence for other screening tools. In 2015, the US Preventive Services Task Force assessed screening for speech and language delay in preschool children and found insufficient evidence for the use of screening instruments in children up to 5 years of age to detect speech and language delay in primary care.8 Conversely, the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends screening for developmental delay using a standardized tool at 9, 18 and 24 or 30 months of age,9 and screening for autism at 18 months and 24 months.10 In Canada, Ontario has implemented an enhanced 18-month well-baby visit, which includes using the Nipissing District Developmental Screen (NDDS) as a surveillance tool to assess for global developmental delay.11,12

More than 20 years have passed since the last Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care guideline on developmental screening, necessitating a new review of the empirical literature to determine the current state of the evidence. The systematic review on which this article is based provided evidence for the task force to inform new recommendations on screening for developmental delay in children aged 1-4 years. This systematic review synthesizes the effectiveness and harms of screening for developmental delay in children with respect to improving cognitive, academic and functional outcomes.

Methods

We conducted a systematic review to address the effectiveness of screening for developmental delay in children aged 1-4 years who are not at risk or suspected of having developmental delay. This search was part of a larger, staged systematic review that investigated treatment of developmental delay and test properties of screening tests (details about the full review protocol are available at www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO; registration no. CRD42014009809). Results from the full review will be available on the Canadian Task Force on Preventive Health Care website (http://canadiantaskforce.ca/). Similar methods have been used by and are reported in other publications authored by our review team.13-15

Key questions

This systematic review aims to answer the following questions: What is the effectiveness of screening children aged 1-4 years without suspected developmental delay to improve their outcomes? What is the optimal interval for screening for developmental delay? What is the incidence of harms of screening children aged 1-4 years without suspected developmental delay?

Search strategy

We searched MEDLINE, Embase and PsychINFO with no beginning date limitations through to June 16, 2015. The published results of studies had to be available in either English or French. The effectiveness of the screening search was peer-reviewed using the PRESS format.16

Eligibility criteria

Details about the inclusion and exclusion criteria are shown in Table 1. Because developmental delay poses a high burden at individual, family and societal levels, rigorous methodologic standards are essential to assess whether developmental screening tools are effective in meaningful ways. For this reason, we included only controlled cohort studies and randomized controlled trials (RCTs) to answer the question of benefits.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1: Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Study selection, quality assessment and data abstraction

Six team members, working in pairs, reviewed titles and abstracts of papers in duplicate; articles marked for inclusion by either member went on to full-text screening, which was performed independently by the same 6 team members. Disagreements were resolved through discussion and consensus.

One team member completed full data abstraction using a Web-based systematic review software program (DistillerSR, Evidence Partners), and a second team member verified this extraction; disagreements were resolved through discussion or by a third team member. All studies included to answer the question about effectiveness of screening were assessed by one team member using the Cochrane risk-of-bias tool18 and verified by another team member. The quality of the evidence was determined based on the Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) methods (www.gradeworkinggroup.org) using GRADEPro software (version 3.2 for Windows, available at http://ims.cochrane.org/revman/other-resources/gradepro/download).

Statistical analysis

We could not perform a meta-analysis because of the paucity of studies. The effect estimates from the studies were reported in the form of relative risk (RR) for binary outcomes of interest such as referral rate and academic performance, and rate ratio for time to event outcomes such as time to referral. For studies that showed a significant effect for the effectiveness of screening for developmental delay, we added the estimates of absolute risk reduction, absolute risk increase and number needed to screen. The numbers needed to screen were estimated using the absolute numbers computed by GRADEpro, which are calculated using control group event rate and relative risk with the 95% confidence interval (CI).

Results

Of the 7782 unique citations identified, 393 were screened in full and 2 studies, both RCTs, were included in our review (Figure 1). The reference lists of 17 identified systematic reviews were searched; no citations were added to our database as a result. Characteristics of the included studies are provided in Table 2.

Figure 1
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1

Selection of studies for the systematic review.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 2: Characteristics of included studies

Effectiveness of screening

Data on effect sizes reported by the studies are shown in Tables 3 and 4, as are our GRADE ratings.

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 3: GRADE evidence profile for the effect of screening for developmental delay in children aged 1-4 years18,19
View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 4: GRADE summary of findings for effect of screening for developmental delay in children 1-4 years old

Referral outcomes

One RCT provided evidence on referral rates and time to referral for children less than 30 months of age who were screened for developmental delay using the Ages and Stages Questionnaire II (ASQ-II).19 This 2013 American study included 2103 boys and girls who were randomly allocated to 1 of 2 intervention groups (screening with office support [group A] or screening without office support [group B]) or usual care. The mean ages were 10.5 (standard devision [SD] 8.2) months in intervention group A, 10.5 (SD 8.1) months in intervention group B and 10.4 (SD 8.6) months in the control group. Families allocated to intervention group A met with trained office staff to complete the screening tool with the use of props; families in intervention group B completed the ASQ-II without support of office staff or the use of props. Children were considered screen positive if they scored 2 SDs below the mean for age on any of the 5 developmental domains, and they could be referred to early intervention services. Children in the control group were considered screen positive if they failed the usual-care developmental screen (milestones of 8-10 questions from 4 domains), and they could also be referred to early intervention services.

There were significantly more referrals to early intervention in intervention group A than in the control group (RR 1.95, 95% CI 1.49-2.54) (Tables 3 and 4). The absolute risk increase was 9.67%, and the number needed to screen for one child to be referred was 10 (95% CI 6-20). The referral rate was also significantly higher in intervention group B than in the control group (RR 1.71, 95% CI 1.30-2.25). The absolute risk increase was 7.24%, and the number needed to screen for one child to be referred was 14 (95% CI 8-33). Compared with the time to referral in the control group, the time was 70% shorter in intervention group A (rate ratio 0.30, 95% CI 0.19-0.48) and 64% shorter in intervention group B (rate ratio 0.36, 95% CI 0.23-0.59).

The GRADE ranking for time to referral and referral rates (for screening with or without office support) was moderate (Tables 3 and 4). Evidence from this study was downgraded for indirectness because participant age at enrolment was less than 12 months.

Academic performance

The other RCT provided data on academic performance in children screened for language delay.20 This 2007 study from the Netherlands included 11 440 boys and girls aged 15 months at enrolment (mean age not reported). Children randomly assigned to the intervention group were screened twice, at 18 and 24 months, with the VroegTijdige Onderkenning Ontwikkelingsstoornissen (VTO) Language Screening instrument; children assigned to the control group received usual monitoring. A final score ranging from 0 to 7 was assigned; children with a total score of 2 or less were referred for additional assessment to confirm language delay. After screening, the study did not offer an intervention and did not indicate whether children received interventions elsewhere.

Assessment of academic performance at age 8 years showed no differences between the study groups in any of the outcomes measured (attendance of a special school: RR 0.71, 95% CI 0.48-1.04; repeating a grade: RR 0.99, 95% CI 0.81-1.21; repeating a grade because of language problems in regular primary school: RR 1.26, 95% CI 0.89-1.80; being below the 10th percentile of oral tests: RR 0.88, 95% CI 0.63-1.23; being below the 10th percentile of reading tests in grade 2: RR 1.00, 95% CI 0.72-1.40; or being below the 10th percentile of spelling tests in grade 2: RR 0.68, 95% CI 0.41-1.13) (Tables 3 and 4).

The GRADE ranking for all outcomes of academic performance was low (Tables 3 and 4). Evidence from this study was downgraded for potential risk of bias because of insufficient information on allocation concealment and blinding of participants, and for imprecision because effect estimates included the null value.

Optimal interval and harms of screening

We found no studies meeting our inclusion criteria that reported on optimal intervals or harms of screening.

Interpretation

The evidence on the effectiveness of screening for developmental delay in improving cognitive, academic and adaptive functioning outcomes in children 1-4 years old is scant. We found one study that reported higher and earlier intervention rates among the children screened for developmental delay than among those in the control group.19 Referral rate is an intermediate outcome; therefore, conclusions about long-term outcomes related to screening and referral to early intervention programs cannot be drawn from this study. The second study reported longer-term follow-up data (81 mo) on academic performance outcomes among children screened at 18 and 24 months for speech and language delay. In this study, screening did not show a significant improvement in academic outcomes at age 8 years. Ideally, the intermediate outcome of early referral leads to early interventions, which then improve long-term outcomes. Unfortunately, our evidence does not confirm this. Furthermore, we found no evidence on which screening intervals are most effective and which result in the least harm.

We did not investigate treatment of developmental delay. As such, we have no evidence on the effectiveness of early intervention programs: the first study investigated only to the point of referral, and the second one did not indicate whether the children received an early intervention program between the initial screen and the 8-year assessment. The evidence on the effectiveness of screening is limited and inconclusive; as such, we cannot comment on the effectiveness of early intervention programs and their impact on cognitive, academic and adaptive functioning.

Currently, no guidelines exist on screening for global developmental delay. The findings of this systematic review are in keeping with the most recent guideline (2015) from the US Preventive Services Task Force, which found insufficient evidence on screening for speech and language delay in children up to 5 years.8

Despite the lack of evidence found in this review and in the US Preventive Services Task Force's systematic review on screening for speech and language in children less than 5 years old,21 screening of children is regularly implemented and endorsed. In the US, the American Academy of Pediatrics recommends screening for developmental delay at regular intervals up to 30 months.9 In Canada, Ontario uses the NDDS as a surveillance tool to monitor for developmental delay in children at the 18-month well-baby visit. Despite common use, we found no peer-reviewed RCT evidence on the NDDS tool. A recent Canadian observational study evaluated the NDDS and found evidence that the tool should not be used on its own (unpublished data). Further investigation into these commonly used tools is required to determine whether their continued use is clinically relevant and appropriate.

Limitations

The inclusion and exclusion criteria for this review limited our results. First, only publications in English or French were considered for inclusion. Second, for the question of effectiveness of screening, only RCTs and controlled cohort studies were included, thus excluding observational studies that may have reported on our outcomes of interest. Although this limited the breadth of evidence available, it ensured a higher quality of evidence. Third, because we selected high-level, long-term outcomes (e.g., cognitive, academic and adaptive functioning), studies reporting on shorter-term, specific outcomes (e.g., changes in expressive or receptive language or changes in social or motor functioning) and on outcomes related to symptoms of autism spectrum disorders were excluded. Based on our exclusion criteria, it is clear that there is research focused on these immediate outcomes, rather than on the long-term outcomes our review aimed to report on. Publication bias and methodologic inconsistency could not be assessed due to the paucity of studies.

Conclusion

The evidence on screening for developmental delay in asymptomatic children aged 1-4 years to improve cognitive, educational and adaptive functioning outcomes is inconclusive. Further research on effectiveness and harms, with longer-term outcomes, is needed to inform decisions about screening and screening intervals.

Supplemental information

For reviewer comments and the original submission of this manuscript, please see www.cmajopen.ca/content/4/1/E20/suppl/DC1

Acknowledgements

The authors are grateful to Sharon Peck-Reid for database management and formatting of the report; Maureen Rice for her screening contributions and for conducting the literature search; Dr. Jessie McGowan for being the peer reviewer of the search strategy; and Drs. Alice Carter, Sharon Smile and Isabel Smith, who peer reviewed the evidence synthesis. Dr. Lesley Dunfield, Scientific Research Manager at the Public Health Agency of Canada (PHAC), contributed to the background chapter as part of original protocol developmental and reviewed drafts of the technical report. Similarly, Drs. Denis Leduc, Patricia Parkin, Kevin Pottie, Brett Thombs and Marcello Tonelli, members of the Developmental Delay Working Group of the Canadian Task Force for Preventive Health Care, provided comments on the protocol and initial analysis and technical report.

Footnotes

  • Competing interests: None declared.

  • Contributors: All of the authors performed tasks involved in conducting the full systematic review. All revised the manuscript critically, approved the final version to be published and agreed to act as guarantors of the work.

  • Funding: Funding for the McMaster Evidence Review and Synthesis Centre was provided by PHAC and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR). The views of the funding bodies did not influence the content of the review or the decision to submit the paper for publication. The views expressed in this article are those of the authors and do not represent those of PHAC or CIHR.

References

  1. ↵
    1. Messinger D,
    2. Young GS,
    3. Ozonoff S,
    4. et al.
    (2013) Beyond autism: a baby siblings research consortium study of high-risk children at three years of age. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 52:300–8.e1.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Campbell F,
    2. Conti G,
    3. Heckman JJ,
    4. et al.
    (2014) Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science 343:1478–85.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Wallace KS,
    2. Rogers SJ
    (2010) Intervening in infancy: implications for autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 51:1300–20.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. American Psychiatric Association.
    (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Fifth edition. Arlington (VA): American Psychiatric Publishing;.
  4. ↵
    1. Feldman W
    (1994) Well-baby care in the first 2 years of life. In: The Canadian Guide to Clinical Preventive Health Care. Ottawa: Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination; pp 258–66.
  5. ↵
    1. Feightner JW
    (1994) Preschool screening for developmental problems. In: The Canadian Guide to Clinical Preventive Health Care. Ottawa: Canadian Task Force on the Periodic Health Examination; pp 289–96.
  6. ↵
    Denver Developmental Screening Test. Denver: Denver Developmental Materials. Available:http://denverii.com/. accessed 2016 Jan. 12.
  7. ↵
    1. Siu AL,
    2. US Preventive Services Task Force
    (2015) Screening for speech and language delay and disorders in children aged 5 years or younger: US Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation Statement. Pediatrics 136:e474–81.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  8. ↵
    1. Council on Children with Disabilities
    (2006) Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics; Bright Futures Steering Committee; Medical Home Initiatives for Children with Special Needs Project Advisory Committee. Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics 118:405–20.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  9. ↵
    1. Johnson CP,
    2. Myers SM
    (2007) Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 120:1183–215.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  10. ↵
    (2005) Getting it right at 18 months ... Making it right for a lifetime. Report of the Expert Panel on the 18 month well baby visit. Toronto: Ontario Children's Health Network; Ontario College of Family Physicians. Available:www.children.gov.on.ca/htdocs/English/topics/earlychildhood/gettingitright.aspx. accessed 2016 Jan. 12.
  11. ↵
    1. NDDS Canada
    (2011) Nipissing district development screen. North Bay (ON): Nipissing District Developmental Screen Intellectual Property Association; Available:www.ndds.ca/canada.html. accessed 2016 Jan. 12.
  12. ↵
    1. Peirson L,
    2. Fitzpatrick-Lewis D,
    3. Morrison K,
    4. et al.
    (2015) Prevention of overweight/obesity in children and youth: a systematic review. CMAJ Open 3:E23–33.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
    1. Peirson L,
    2. Douketis J,
    3. Ciliska D,
    4. et al.
    (2014) Prevention of overweight and obeisty in adult populations: a systematic review. CMAJ Open 2:E268–72.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  13. ↵
    1. Peirson L,
    2. Douketis J,
    3. Ciliska D,
    4. et al.
    (2014) Treatment for overweight and obesity in adult populations: a systematic reivew and meta-analysis. CMAJ Open 2:E306–17.
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  14. ↵
    1. McGowan J,
    2. Sampson M,
    3. Lefebvre C
    (2010) An evidence based checklist for the peer review of electronic search strategies (PRESS EBC). Evid Based Libr Inf Pract 5:149–54.
    OpenUrl
  15. (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed. Washington (DC): American Psychiatric Association;.
  16. ↵
    1. Higgins JPT,
    2. Green S
    (2011) editorsCochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions. Version 5.1.0. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Available:www.cochrane-handbook.org. accessed 2016 Jan. 12.
  17. ↵
    1. Guevara JP,
    2. Gerdes M,
    3. Localio R,
    4. et al.
    (2013) Effectiveness of developmental screening in an urban setting. Pediatrics 131:30–7.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  18. ↵
    1. van Agt HME,
    2. van der Stege HA,
    3. de Ridder-Sluiter H,
    4. et al.
    (2007) A cluster-randomized trial of screening for language delay in toddlers: effects on school performance and language development at age 8. Pediatrics 120:1317–25.
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  19. ↵
    1. Berkman ND,
    2. Wallace I,
    3. Watson L,
    4. et al.
    (2015) Screening for speech and language delays and disorders in children age 5 years or younger: a systematic review for the U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. [Report no. 13-05197-EF-1]. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Available: www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26225412. accessed 2016 Jan. 12.
  • Copyright 2016, 8872147 Canada Inc.
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

CMAJ Open: 4 (1)
Vol. 4, Issue 1
30 Mar 2016
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author

Article tools

Respond to this article
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
To sign up for email alerts or to access your current email alerts, enter your email address below:
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in spreading the word on CMAJ Open.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Screening for developmental delay among children aged 1-4 years: a systematic review
(Your Name) has sent you a message from CMAJ Open
(Your Name) thought you would like to see the CMAJ Open web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Screening for developmental delay among children aged 1-4 years: a systematic review
Rachel Warren, Meghan Kenny, Teresa Bennett, Donna Fitzpatrick-Lewis, Muhammad Usman Ali, Diana Sherifali, Parminder Raina
Jan 2016, 4 (1) E20-E27; DOI: 10.9778/cmajo.20140121

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Screening for developmental delay among children aged 1-4 years: a systematic review
Rachel Warren, Meghan Kenny, Teresa Bennett, Donna Fitzpatrick-Lewis, Muhammad Usman Ali, Diana Sherifali, Parminder Raina
Jan 2016, 4 (1) E20-E27; DOI: 10.9778/cmajo.20140121
Digg logo Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Google logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like

Related Articles

  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • Edition 2020 du Releve postnatal Rourke: Quoi de neuf en soins preventifs des enfants jusqua 5 ans?
  • 2020 edition of the Rourke Baby Record: What is new in preventive care of children up to 5 years of age?
  • Screening tools for early identification of children with developmental delay in low- and middle-income countries: a systematic review
  • Google Scholar

Similar Articles

Collections

  • Clinical
    • Pediatrics
      • Children
      • Infants
      • Other pediatrics
    • Mental health
  • Nonclinical
    • Epidemiology
      • Screening tests

Content

  • Current issue
  • Past issues
  • Collections
  • Alerts
  • RSS

Authors & Reviewers

  • Overview for Authors
  • Preparing manuscripts
  • Manuscript Submission Checklist
  • Publication Fees
  • Forms
  • Editorial Policies
  • Editorial Process
  • Patient-Oriented Research
  • Submit a manuscript
  • Manuscript Progress
  • Submitting a letter
  • Information for Reviewers

About

  • General Information
  • Staff
  • Editorial Board
  • Advisory Panel
  • Contact Us
  • Advertising
  • Media
  • Reprints
  • Copyright and Permissions
  • Accessibility
  • CMA Civility Standards
CMAJ Group

Copyright 2023, CMA Impact Inc. or its licensors. All rights reserved. ISSN 2291-0026

All editorial matter in CMAJ OPEN represents the opinions of the authors and not necessarily those of the Canadian Medical Association or its subsidiaries.

To receive any of these resources in an accessible format, please contact us at CMAJ Group, 500-1410 Blair Towers Place, Ottawa ON, K1J 9B9; p: 1-888-855-2555; e: [email protected].

View CMA's Accessibility policy.

 

Powered by HighWire